Study shows differences in rapidly processing sensory feedback among
people with autism spectrum disorder
Date:
August 11, 2021
Source:
University of Kansas
Summary:
In an effort to better characterize -- and quantify -- sensorimotor
abilities and their impact on the development of people with ASD,
scientists have published research that compared the fine motor
control and eye movements of more than 200 people from age 5 to
29. The study showed key differences in the ways that individuals
with ASD processed or reacted to stimuli compared to those who
were more neurotypical, with implications for future research
across age range, intelligence and setting.
FULL STORY ========================================================================== Sensorimotor issues aren't well understood in people with autism
spectrum disorder, or ASD, yet they are important because they can be
observed before social and communication indicators for the disorder,
and they can predict worse functional outcomes. Such skills can affect everything from handwriting to zipping up a coat to language development,
with implications for education and independence over a person's lifetime.
==========================================================================
In an effort to better characterize -- and quantify -- sensorimotor
abilities and their impact on the development of people with ASD, a group
of University of Kansas scientists has published research that compared
the fine motor control and eye movements of more than 200 people from age
5 to 29. The study showed key differences in the ways that individuals
with ASD processed or reacted to stimuli compared to those who were more neurotypical, with implications for future research across age range, intelligence and setting.
The research included a precision grip force test that prompted
individuals to squeeze their thumb and forefinger together while reacting
to objects on a monitor, and tests that measured eye movements in response
to dots appearing on the screen. Data collected included reaction time, accuracy, variability and consistency.
The research was as significant for what it documented as well as how,
compared to other studies that look at sensorimotor issues in isolation,
said lead author Kathryn Unruh, assistant research professor at the
Kansas Center for Autism Research and Treatment at the KU Life Span
Institute. It included individuals across a broad age range, allowing
the team to take a more fine- grained approach to examining sensorimotor behaviors and how they are affected across development.
"Precision grip and eye movement are supported by separate brain
systems that utilize sensory and motor information in different ways,"
she said. "By measuring features of motor behavior across both of
these systems, we're able to develop a more complete picture of what sensorimotor deficits look like in ASD and how those may vary across individuals." The study confirmed previous KU and other findings showing
that ASD can affect the ability to rapidly process sensory feedback and translate changes in information into a precise motor movement. To do so,
the study explored two systems that interact to produce sensorimotor
behaviors: feedback and feed- forward systems. Feedback systems are
involved in perceiving and using sensory information, like visual or
tactile information, to produce motor movements that can be dynamically adjusted over time. Feed-forward systems are responsible for carrying out actions that are very rapid and happen too quickly for sensory feedback
to be processed in the brain.
"For example, when you reach out to grab a cup of coffee, a feed-forward situation, you don't think about it," Unruh said. "The brain initiates
this movement based on the many times you reached for the coffee
before. Conversely, feedback systems are responsible for monitoring to
see if any adjustments needed to be made to that plan -- maybe it's
a little further away than you thought, or the mug is heavier than
you expected. Both of these systems work together to make precise,
accurate, and effective movements that allow you to pick up your coffee
without spilling it." A primary finding from the study is that timing
seems to matter when it comes to sensorimotor deficits in ASD. When
motor adjustments needed to be made very rapidly, deficits were more
pronounced in people with ASD, while when motor processes occurred
over a relatively longer period of time, smaller differences were seen
between ASD and age-matched neurotypical peers. One interpretation for
these findings is that people with ASD may rely more heavily on slower
feedback processes than neurotypical peers.
The results also indicated that precision motor deficits in ASD vary
across ages and contexts. For example, the study found that precision
grip movements at very low force, similar to the degree of movement that
a person uses to perform daily living tasks such as holding a pencil or buttoning a shirt, were profoundly affected in very young children with
ASD relative to their neurotypical peers. That data in turn suggests
that measuring precision grip variability at a low force level could
help reliably differentiate children with ASD from typically developing children, with potential for early intervention.
Additional observations in the study focused on differences in force
across "handedness," or from dominant hand to non-dominant hand. In
typically developing individuals, scientists expect to see stronger force exerted from a dominant hand, but in people with ASD, they observed
less difference between the dominant and non-dominant hand, which
suggests reduced specialization between hemispheres of the brain. Reduced hemispheric specialization can impact development of other key cognitive
and behavioral abilities associated with ASD, such as language.
The research reflects that scientists need to look across multiple
systems to understand sensorimotor issues in people with ASD, Unruh said.
The research was published in the journal Molecular Autism and was
co-written by Walker McKinney, Erin Bojanek, Kandace Fleming and Matt
Mosconi, all from KU, and John Sweeney of the University of Cincinnati.
========================================================================== Story Source: Materials provided by University_of_Kansas. Note: Content
may be edited for style and length.
========================================================================== Journal Reference:
1. Kathryn E. Unruh, Walker S. McKinney, Erin K. Bojanek, Kandace K.
Fleming, John A. Sweeney, Matthew W. Mosconi. Initial
action output and feedback-guided motor behaviors in autism
spectrum disorder. Molecular Autism, 2021; 12 (1) DOI:
10.1186/s13229-021-00452-8 ==========================================================================
Link to news story:
https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2021/08/210811162908.htm
--- up 13 weeks, 5 days, 22 hours, 45 minutes
* Origin: -=> Castle Rock BBS <=- Now Husky HPT Powered! (1:317/3)