• Mapping words to colors

    From ScienceDaily@1:317/3 to All on Thu Sep 23 21:30:38 2021
    Mapping words to colors

    Date:
    September 23, 2021
    Source:
    University of Pennsylvania
    Summary:
    While the range of colors your eyes may perceive extends beyond
    the words language provides, languages around the globe are
    remarkably similar in how they partition the space of colors into a
    vocabulary. Yet differences exist. In a study examining 130 diverse
    languages around the world, researchers developed an algorithm to
    infer the communicative needs that different linguistic communities
    place on colors.



    FULL STORY ==========================================================================
    No language has words for all the blues of a wind-churned sea or the
    greens and golds of a wildflower meadow in late summer. Globally,
    different languages have divvied up the world of color using their own
    set of labels, from just a few to dozens.


    ==========================================================================
    The question of how humans have done this -- ascribe a finite vocabulary
    to the multitude of perceivable colors -- has been long studied, and
    consistent patterns have emerged, even across wildly divergent languages
    and cultures. Yet slight differences among languages persist, and what
    is less understood is how the differing communicative needs of local
    cultures drive those differences. Do some cultures need to talk about
    certain colors more than others, and how does that shape their language?
    In a new study, researchers led by Colin Twomey, a postdoc in Penn's
    MindCORE program, and Joshua Plotkin, a professor in the School of Arts
    & Sciences' Biology Department, address these questions, developing an algorithm capable of inferring a culture's communicative needs -- the imperative to talk about certain colors -- using previously collected
    data from 130 diverse languages.

    Their findings underscore that, indeed, cultures across the globe differ
    in their need to communicate about certain colors. Linking almost all languages, however, is an emphasis on communicating about warm colors
    -- reds and yellows -- that are known to draw the human eye and that
    correspond with the colors of ripe fruits in primate diets.

    The work, a collaboration that included Penn linguist Gareth Roberts and psychologist David Brainard, is published in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

    "The fact that color vocabularies could be an efficient representation of
    the communicative needs of colors is an idea that's been around for 20
    years," says Twomey. "It struck me that, OK, if this is our idea about
    how color vocabularies are formed, then we could go in reverse and ask,
    'Well, what would have been the communicative needs that would have
    been necessary for this vocabulary to arrive at its present form?' It's
    a hard problem, but I had an intuition that it was a solvable one."
    "The color-word problem is a classical one: How do you map the infinitude
    of colors to a discrete number of words?" says Plotkin. "Colin noticed
    an evolutionary interpretation of the problem. It's as if the different
    terms are competing for what colors they will be used to represent. That
    was a key mathematical insight that allows us to infer the communicative
    needs of colors in each of these 130 languages."


    ==========================================================================
    The study relied on a robust dataset known as the World Color Survey,
    collected more than 50 years ago by anthropologist Brent Berlin and
    linguist Paul Kay.

    Traveling to 130 linguistic communities worldwide, Berlin and Kay
    presented native speakers with the same 330 color chips. They found that
    even completely different languages tended to group colors in roughly
    the same way. What's more, when they asked speakers to identify the focal
    color of a particular named color -- the "reddest red" or "greenest green"
    -- speakers' choices were highly similar across languages.

    "Their results were so astonishing," Plotkin says. "They demanded
    explanation." Substantial research followed, some of which suggested
    that one major reason for the remarkable similarities between languages'
    color vocabularies came down to physiology.

    "Languages differ, cultures differ, but our eyes are the same," says
    Plotkin.

    But another reason for the overarching similarities could be that humans, regardless of what language they speak, are more interested in talking
    about certain colors than others.



    ==========================================================================
    The Penn team used data from the World Color Survey on focal colors to
    work backwards, going from speakers' observations of the reddest red or greenest green to infer the communicative need associated with each of
    the 330 colors in the survey.

    "What was really surprising was that we could use just those best-example colors to say what those communicative needs would have been," says
    Twomey.

    The researchers were able to use the second part of the World Color Survey data, on how languages divided color, to validate that their inference algorithm could predict the communicative needs of different languages.

    Their analysis underscores findings from earlier research, that warm-hued colors have a higher communicative need. "On average across languages,
    the reds and yellows have 30-fold greater demand than other colors,"
    Plotkin says.

    "No one really cares about brownish greens, and pastels aren't super
    well represented in communicative needs," Twomey adds.

    The researchers also looked at existing data on fruit-eating primates
    with color vision systems like our own. These primates tend to eat
    ripe fruit with colors that line up almost precisely with the places
    in the color spectrum with high communicative need. "Fruits are a way
    for a plant to spread its seeds, hitching a ride with the animals that
    eat them. Fruit-producing plants likely evolved to stand out to these
    animals. The relationship with the colors of ripe fruit tells us that communicative needs are likely related to the colors that stand out
    to us the most," says Twomey. "To be clear, this doesn't say that we
    have the communicative needs we have because we need to communicate
    about fruit specifically." The team's algorithm could predict not only
    the similarities but also the differences between languages. While an
    emphasis on reds and yellows was universal, certain languages also had
    high communicative needs for blues, while greens turned up as important
    in other languages. The research team found that some of these differences
    were associated with biogeography and distance.

    Cultures that shared similar ecoregions were more similar in their communicative needs around colors, perhaps owing to plants or animals
    in that region that were important for food or other uses.

    This approach to the study of communicative needs opens up many other
    areas for study. "This is something that could be carried to other systems where there is a need to divide up some cognitive space," says Twomey,
    "whether it's sound, weight, temperature, or something else." And using
    this framework also opens up opportunities for pursuing various questions around color and language.

    "Now that we have inferred how often people want to talk about certain
    colors today, we can take a phylogeny of languages and try to infer
    what people were talking about 500 or 1,000 years ago. What historical
    events coincide with changes in our needs to talk about colors?" Plotkin
    says. "There is tons of work still to be done here." Such questions
    will demand unique collaborations like the one undergirded by MindCORE,
    a campus hub for study of human intelligence and behavior which enabled
    this work. "Inherently interdisciplinary questions like the ones we
    tackle in our paper together can be challenging to work on precisely
    because it takes a team of experts from different fields to answer
    them," Twomey says. "So I feel very fortunate to have had MindCORE's
    support here at Penn to assemble exactly the right team for this problem." ========================================================================== Story Source: Materials provided by University_of_Pennsylvania. Note:
    Content may be edited for style and length.


    ========================================================================== Journal Reference:
    1. Colin R. Twomey, Gareth Roberts, David H. Brainard, Joshua
    B. Plotkin.

    What we talk about when we talk about colors. Proceedings of the
    National Academy of Sciences, 2021; 118 (39): e2109237118 DOI:
    10.1073/ pnas.2109237118 ==========================================================================

    Link to news story: https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2021/09/210923153349.htm

    --- up 3 weeks, 8 hours, 25 minutes
    * Origin: -=> Castle Rock BBS <=- Now Husky HPT Powered! (1:317/3)